How Newton’s experiment is often wrongly shown. |
The writing diary of a well-mellowed science writer who cares about the public understanding of science and knows the ropes. This blog bounces between my curiosity, the daily realities of professional writing, the joy of pursuing nature, and my recycling of ideas that won't be in some book or other as far as I can see, but still needed sharing. I welcome comments and suggestions! Spam will be blocked and reported. For my books, see http://members.ozemail.com.au/~macinnis/writing/index.htm
Search This Blog
Thursday, 22 April 2021
Different light: Newton and the spectrum
Monday, 19 April 2021
A Different Brain
This is the first part of They Saw The Difference, announced here.
Dart’s original illustration of the Taung child, 1925. |
I have always told my students that the best actors go into law, the next best become teachers, and the leftovers go to stage and screen. The reader may justly conclude from this that when I teach (or write) I’m putting on a show.
About 1992, I prepared for work each day by slipping the
fossilised toe-bone of a giant kangaroo into my shirt pocket, a child’s brain
into one trouser pocket and its skull into the other.
<SFX> Brakes screech, voices off, shouting “Wha-a-at?”
To clarify, the skull and brain were fossils, 2 to 3 million
years old, plus or minus a bit, and if the kangaroo toe-bone was real, museums
around the world make casts of their best and rarest examples to sell to other
museums, so what you see in a glass case or a lecturer’s hand is usually a
copy, cast in resin from a mould of the original, and painted to resemble the
original, which is somewhere safe. No matter, just having casts of the skull
and part of its brain in my pockets allowed me to tell their story, as well as
if I held the genuine relics.
Raymond Dart was
an Australian teaching in South Africa in the 1920s. In 1924, he received two
boxes of rocks on a morning when he was supposed to be getting ready to act as
best man to his friend, Christo Beyers. Peeking into one of the boxes, he saw the cast of a brain lying loose, and in that, he saw something important.
Soon after the
brain’s owner died, mud partly filled a skull, and this mud later hardened to rock.
Technically, it was an endocast, a copy of part of the inside of the skull
which closely reflected the brain, but it wasn’t any old brain—it was special,
because of its small size and the position of its brain stem.
Interpreting
fossils is an art and a science. Experts must know anatomy, how the parts work
together, what small differences mean, and they work with those small
differences. The position of the large hole in the skull where the spinal cord
leaves the brain, the foramen magnum,
was immediately obvious in the shape of the brain. Any animal with a brain stem
like that had to have walked upright.
We cannot be certain how the Taung child died, but clearly the skull had ended up on its side in a lime-rich deposit, where the brain case was slightly more than half-filled with the mud which became the cast.
Dart saw that it fitted into a
block of stone in the case, so a major part of the brain owner’s skull was probably
there as well. He was a medical man, but fascinated by fossils, and he knew
that this was important. So was his friend's wedding, but afterwards, he itched to get back to his find.
The covering rock
had to be carefully removed before the face could be examined, but a quick look
at the cast was all Dart needed. The brain said this animal had a skull which
attached to a vertical spine, lying directly below the skull, rather than
behind it, as in chimpanzees and gorillas. The owner walked upright, like
modern humans. Here is how Dart worked it out:
I was also convinced from the earliest period of my
investigations that these creatures had placed great reliance on their feet for
walking and running and that, consequently, their hands must have been freed
for other tasks. This was implicit in the globular form of the skull which was
obviously balanced on a more vertically placed type of backbone than that of a
gorilla or chimpanzee. The improvement in the poise of the head implied a
better posture of the whole body framework, since there must have been a
relative forward displacement of the foramen magnum (the hole in the
base of the skull which links the brain with the spinal cord).
—Raymond Dart, Adventures with the Missing Link, 1959, 11.
The people who interpret fossils work like Sherlock Holmes
at his best. To those who can read, a glimpse of a document can be enough, but
those who can read fossils can gain just as much from a single glimpse of just
the right hint. At this point, Dart made a political mistake.
Even in the 1920s, a careful observer would have seen that the British Empire was already in decay, and there were few careful observers around, but there was a cast-iron rule: London is always right. When Dart reported his find in Nature in 1925, London came down on him like a ton of bricks.
His find (known as the “Taung child”, from
where it was found and its obvious youthfulness) was small-brained and most
British scientists were certain that any small-brained thing was no ancestor of
theirs. Piltdown Man was the human beginning, they said: he had a big brain,
and best of all, he was found in Britain! (There’s more on Piltdown in the
Afterword, but you'll have to get the book to read that.)
Today, we might
think Dart’s name for his find, Australopithecus
(“southern ape”), was not the best name for an upright-walking individual, even
one with a small brain, but Dart was trying not to draw too much fire upon
himself. It didn’t work, but in the long run, the brain stem evidence held up
and Piltdown was eventually shown to be a fake.
The true status of
the Taung child lay hidden inside its jaw until 1987. In both humans and the
other apes, the “adult” teeth emerge in a specific sequence. There is one order
of appearance in humans, and a different order of tooth eruption in the other
apes. Concealed inside the Taung child’s skull, teeth were erupting, and their
pattern of development would tell us what the Taung child was, either human or ape. As
there is only one Taung child, you cannot slice it up, just to see what is
inside. You could take X-rays, but there is too much other material in the way,
and the things we are looking for are much too faint.
For many years, it
seemed as though we would never know what was inside the jaw. Then in 1987,
Glenn Conroy and Michael Vannier had a bright idea. Instead of cutting the
skull into thin slices, they made a series of virtual slices with X-rays, and
fed the results into a computer, and used back projection to build up a
three-dimensional picture of what was inside. Seeing how the Taung baby’s teeth
were erupting would give the answer.
The researchers
took their X-ray shots, just 2 mm apart, in three different dimensions:
vertically, from front to back, vertically, from side to side, and
horizontally. (They called it the sagittal, coronal and transaxial planes, if
you prefer the technicalities.) The method is less important, but the answer
was delightful:
…the Taung ‘child’ is not a little human, but just as
important, it is not a little ape…
— Glenn C. Conroy & Michael W. Vannier, Nature 329, 625–627, 21 October 1987.
The whole answer was told in the differences: the Taung baby
is a betwixt-and-between, a half-and-half, a missing link if you wish, and we
would never have known if the two researchers had not decided to give it a CAT
scan! Sadly, we had to wait another sixty years to find out what it was.
The story I told,
over several years at the Australian Museum, was about how Dart saw a
difference, and recognised a new scientific truth. This was just a few years
after Conroy and Vannier had confirmed the role the Taung child’s people played
in our origins, but there was more: I had human and gorilla skulls, that toe
bone of the giant kangaroo and the matching bone from a horse. Always, it was
about differences.
At other times, I
talked to my audience about Edward Tyson (1651 – 1708), one of the unsung heroes of science,
who persuaded Robert Hooke to pay seven shillings and sixpence for a 43 kg
porpoise from a London fishmonger, so Tyson could dissect it. Back then, even experts like John Ray called the porpoise a fish,
but Tyson’s Anatomy of a Porpess,
published in 1680 showed the danger of judging a book by its cover. He said:
“If we view a Porpess on the outside, there is nothing more than a Fish, but if
we look within, there is nothing less.”
Tyson later
dissected an infant chimpanzee which had died after being brought to London
from Angola. While he referred to it as both a ‘pygmie’ and an ‘Orang-Outang’,
the drawings show a chimpanzee, but Tyson’s book, filled with illustrations, showed
for the first time just how close humans were to the other animals, and how
they differed.
If Copernicus had
removed the earth from the centre of the universe (something I describe in chapter 10), Tyson and his
assistant, William Cowper, helped to remove Homo
sapiens from a central position in creation. This change tied together
humans and the whole of ‘lower’ creation. Tyson had taken one of the crucial
steps towards recognising that evolution happened.
Next, back to
Newton again…
Sunday, 18 April 2021
They saw the difference
I am, first and foremost, an historian of science, and I'm going to talk for the next month or so about my new e-book, soon to be a print-on-demand book, called They Saw the Difference. The thing is, I've been busy getting our block of townhouses repainted and rejigging a couple of older titles. so for now, here's the introduction to They Saw the Difference.
I keep six honest serving-men
(They taught me all I knew);
Their names are What and Why and When,
And How and Where and Who.
—Rudyard Kipling, introduction to ‘The Elephant’s Child’ in the Just So
Stories.
Differences, seeking, cultivating and studying them, make our civilisation work. The art of noting and celebrating differences bloomed in Renaissance Europe, and detecting differences shaped modern science and technology, but the habit was there long ago.
The early hominin who saw that this rock was better than that rock for forming tools, or observed that wood burned and rocks did not, the one who noticed that water ran downhill and not up, these were the ancestors of modern scientists and technologists.
My granddaughters seeing the difference an echidna makes. |
As the subtitle says, this is a social history of science, concentrating on the why and the how, with a good dollop of what, and something of the who, where and when, along with regular bursts of something completely different. This book compulsively pursues puzzles to their ends.
For example engineers and physicists, hunting for scraps of literary
icing to decorate their published work often quote these words of Paul Ambroise
Valéry (1871 – 1945): “One had to be a
Newton to notice that the moon is falling, when everyone sees that it doesn’t
fall.”
If the quoters offer a source (most of them don't), it has a
date of 1970, which is well after the poet’s death. By enlisting the burrowing skills of
Project Wombat, I know that their 1970 source is volume 14 of Valéry’s
posthumous collected works, but the quote was first published as “Il fallait être Newton pour apercevoir que
la lune tombe, quand tout le monde voit bien qu’elle ne tombe pas,” in Mélange, Grandeurs, 384, Oeuvres, t. 1, La Pléiade, in 1939. I
sweat the details to get the backstory.
“Is there any point to which you
would wish to draw my attention?”
“To the curious incident of the dog in the night-time.”
“The dog did nothing in the night-time.”
“That was the curious incident,” remarked Sherlock Holmes.
—Arthur Conan Doyle, ‘Silver Blaze’, in The Memoirs of Sherlock Holmes.
Valéry knew what was going down when Newton’s apple fell. He
didn’t imagine crazy young Isaac, sitting under a tree, thinking “apple,
falling: that’s odd!”. Newton was differently equipped, mentally speaking, but
he knew his apples. To him, the odd thing wasn’t the falling apple, it was the
curious way the moon failed ever to reach Earth. That was his dog that didn’t
bark in the night.
He saw that the moon’s orbit involved a fall that went on forever (in
our time frame), the descent always cancelled out by the satellite’s forward motion.
That was the difference he saw, a whole branch of science sprang from it, and
Paul Valéry could see that. We will return to Isaac Newton again soon, because
he could see differences, and he also made a difference.
I chose to follow, not the broad highways of science, thronged by the
famous and important, but rather to stray down the alleys and dusty tracks,
where the interesting people and the curious science lie in wait for us. I have
enjoyed making this work, written for the child I once was and still am: I hope you find
some of the same joy.
Two roads diverged in a wood, and
I—
I took the one less traveled by,
And that has made all the difference.
— Robert Frost, The Road Not Taken.
Monday, 5 April 2021
Back in the saddle
Well, the past three months have been a bit of a rush. I finished editing and compiling Old Grandpa's Book of Practical Poems, mainly for my grandchildren, and that was going to be it, but there was an old ms, lingering on my hard disc, a novel with the working title Sheep May Safely Craze. That is now (as of this morning) locked up and being submitted.
No sooner had I got into the sheep than I attended a kid's lit function, and before I go on, I need to comment. An over-rated novelist once said in a radio interview that he might write for children, “but only if I had brain damage”. This got right up the noses of children’s writers everywhere, because those of us who write for the young know that our craft is far more challenging than writing for adults.
True, the occasional B-grade royal or C-grade celebrity may “write a book for children” (usually meaning they had it ghost-written), but the sales of “their” fulsome drivel will usually relate only to the alleged author’s notoriety.
Such works are
stereotyped, devoid of intellectual commitment or literary value. No matter, we
serious and devoted scriveners keep on engaging young minds, turning on the
lights, although I sometimes take a break and write a book for adult readers,
if there’s a story there that will feed older minds. The book I describe below is just such a
case.
The trapped echidna |
The freed echidna |
Over coffee afterwards, three friends asked me, separately, and within
the space of a couple of minutes, if I was doing a book on echidnas. My answers
were, respectively, “Naaah”, “Maybe” and “You betcha!”
My third interlocutor started out by assuming I would
say yes, and before I could answer, she had reminded me that most children’s
books about echidnas are cloying, saccharine tales of how an anthropomorphic Eddie the echidna couldn’t play with
balloons. Those books aren’t about echidnas, they’re about overcoming
disabilities, and while that’s socially useful, those books don’t advance
understanding or inspire curiosity.
I had already decided that editing a poetry anthology for youngsters and completing a social history of science for oldsters would see me ready to hang up my pen and retire to gardening, leavened by watching noisy action movies and reading Proust, Joyce, P. D. James, Andrea Camilleri and other quality murder mysteries. Instead, I succumbed to peer pressure and launched into this
(Literary social climbers will be pleased to know that Proust and
Joyce return in cameo roles in chapter 10, though this may be seen as a cunning
ploy to convert certain library costs into tax deductions.)
Going home on the ferry, I started making notes, and I soon
realised I would have to read a lot of technical stuff, but there was a story
there, waiting to be told, and young readers would like it. Echidnas, spiny
anteaters, porcupine anteaters (or Tachyglossus
aculeatus if you have my sort of training), have odd quirks. My notes, my
initial thoughts, included the following headings, all later went into my
planning spreadsheet, and here they are:
* spiky, not at all cuddly;
* not really warm, lay eggs, suckle young:
* many scientific names;
* mainly solitary;
* good diggers (claws!);
* fossils, platypus relatives, Zaglossus;
* Sydney 2000 Olympic mascots: echidna, platypus and kookaburra;
* five-cent coin, postage stamp:
* echidna trains;
* do they drink water?
Over the next fortnight, my plan began to change, because in
one week, Christine and I saw four different echidnas, and in the five days
around Christmas 2020, we saw three more, and different, echidnas. Then when I
started looking at the scientific literature, I realised the really good story
was too complex for young readers.
My initial plan for an intellectually honest, stereotype-free, factual book for
youngsters had to go on hold. Having
declared to friends and family that echidnas (working title) is to be my Last Book, I may still
come back to do a simpler version for youngsters, because we still don’t have
all the answers, and that’s a good thing for young people (of all ages) to
know.
In this book, you will find heaps of technical stuff
about physiology, chromosomes, parasites, embryos, membranes, teeth and more. I
promise one thing, though: as a children’s writer, I take all the facts, one at
a time, and make each give a sound account of itself, but there will only be
facts. There will be no flights of fantasy like one I found in Blazing Passion, the book a friend from
Project Wombat passed on, complete with this blurb:
…a
breathtaking romance that races from the turbulence of nineteenth-century
England to the sweltering penal colonies in the Australian jungle…
The book in
question was published by Playboy Books, and ‘Stephanie Blake’ is in reality two men who clearly know very
little about Australia (“sweltering penal colonies in the Australian jungle”?),
but, one assumes, given their publisher, know lots about erotic fantasy. Should
you want a copy, Blazing Passion
came out in 1978. To save your time, here’s a sample of what passes for
dialogue there:
“I’ll fix you up a proper feast. Platypus eggs. Bacon.
Sausage and pancakes. And real coffee …”
Actually, I do offer one flight of fantasy later on in the book, but it
is clearly fanciful. Finding it is something I leave to the reader, but it’s
not the bit about socks full of sea urchins. Those are totally real, and also rate a mention in Sheep.
But that's another story.
Actually, what is a whole 'nother story is that I am resuming control of my out-of-print works and republishing under the Amazon Print-on-demand system.
More on that, later...